Mind-Wandering in the Age of Overstimulation: The Mental Health Impact of Boredom
Posted on April 12th, 2025
Dr. Ransirini de Silva & Dr. Ruwan M. Jayatunge

Boredom is an underexplored but significant emotional state with implications for mental health. In the modern digital age—where attention is continually stimulated and information is instantaneously accessible—the experience of boredom may be more frequent, and perhaps more distressing, than in previous generations.
Boredom is a transient affective state commonly perceived as dull or unpleasant. O’Hanlon (1981) described it as arising from monotonous tasks or limited external stimulation, while Barbalet (1999) conceptualized it as a discrete emotional experience. Typically regarded as negative (Danckert et al., 2018), boredom prompts individuals to seek novelty or escape the perceived stagnation, often resulting in mind-wandering and restlessness.
Universally, boredom is defined as the aversive state of wanting, but being unable, to engage in satisfying activity (Eastwood et al., 2012). Deng et al. (2022) associate boredom with self-dysregulation and attentional failure. It has also been linked to increased nostalgia (van Tilburg et al., 2012) and may reflect a lack of progress toward personally meaningful goals (van Hooft et al., 2018) or meaningful relationships, accompanied by diminished control over one’s life (Steele et al., 2013).
Despite its ubiquity, boredom remains understudied in the field of mental health (Bench & Lench, 2013). It is often identified as a symptom of diminished meaning or purpose (Binnema, 2004) and a precursor to impulsive behaviors (Dittmar & Drury, 2000). Associations have been found between boredom and substance misuse (Lee et al., 2006), problem gambling (Mercer et al., 2010), and reduced performance in academic and occupational settings (Li et al., 2024). Students experiencing boredom may underperform, while employees facing job boredom may suffer decreased productivity and deteriorating well-being.
Importantly, boredom has also been associated with serious psychological concerns. It is recognized as a risk factor for anxiety and depression (Olié et al., 2022) and contributes to poor psychological well-being more broadly (Weiss et al., 2022). It is closely intertwined with experiences of loneliness and social isolation (An et al., 2013) and may contribute to post-psychotic mood disturbances (Todman, 2003).
The COVID-19 pandemic further underscored the psychological impact of boredom. Prolonged social isolation and service disruptions heightened feelings of stagnation and meaninglessness. Tam et al. (2023) suggest that individuals who perceived boredom negatively experienced greater psychological vulnerability during this period.
Yet, boredom is not inherently detrimental. Emerging research reveals its adaptive potential when approached reflectively. Carroll et al. (2010) suggest that boredom can stimulate challenge-seeking, creativity, and prosocial behavior. Bench et al. (2012) argue that it encourages the pursuit of new, more fulfilling goals. When reframed as a signal for reorientation—rather than simply avoided—boredom can support resilience and psychological growth.
In summary, boredom is a complex, underappreciated emotional state with clear mental health consequences. It is associated with psychological distress, impulsivity, and diminished well-being, yet it may also motivate meaningful change and adaptive engagement. As digital environments reshape how we experience attention and engagement, understanding boredom becomes increasingly relevant. Future research and clinical practice must consider how to both mitigate its risks and cultivate its transformative potential—by encouraging rest, embracing boredom as a reflective state, and integrating boredom-tolerance into resilience-building programs.
Dr. Ransirini de Silva PhD is a Senior Lecturer in Psychology and a Clinical Psychologist. She is the Head /Psychology & Counselling Department of Psychology & Counselling, Faculty of Health Sciences, The Open University of Sri Lanka.
Dr. Ruwan M. Jayatunge M.D. PhD is a Medical Doctor and a Clinical Psychologist, also a member of the (APA) American Psychological Association. He is a guest lecturer at Sri Lankan and North American universities.
References
An, J., Payne, L. L., Lee, M., & Janke, M. C. (2023). Understanding boredom and leisure in later life: A systematic review. Innovation in Aging, 7(8), igad109. https://doi.org/10.1093/geroni/igad109
Barbalet, J. M. (1999). Boredom and social meaning. The British Journal of Sociology, 50(4), 631–646. https://doi.org/10.1080/000713199358572
Bench, S. W., & Lench, H. C. (2013). On the function of boredom. Behavioral Sciences, 3(3), 459–472. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs3030459
Bench, S. W., & Lench, H. C. (2019). Boredom as a seeking state: Boredom prompts the pursuit of novel (even negative) experiences. Emotion, 19(2), 242–254. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000433.
Binnema, D. (2004). Interrelations of psychiatric patient experiences of boredom and mental health. Issues in Mental Health Nursing, 25(8), 833–842. https://doi.org/10.1080/01612840490506400.
Carroll, B. J., Parker, P., & Inkson, K. (2010). Evasion of boredom: An unexpected spur to leadership? Human Relations, 63(7), 1031–1049. https://doi.org/10.1177/0018726709349864.
Danckert, J., & Merrifield, C. (2018). Boredom, sustained attention and the default mode network. Experimental Brain Research, 236(9), 2507–2518. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-016-4617-5.
Deng, Y. Q., Shi, G., Zhang, B., Zheng, X., Liu, Y., Zhou, C., & Wang, X. (2022). The effect of mind wandering on cognitive flexibility is mediated by boredom. Acta Psychologica, 231, 103789. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2022.103789.
Eastwood, J. D., Frischen, A., Fenske, M. J., & Smilek, D. (2012). The unengaged mind: Defining boredom in terms of attention. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7(5), 482–495. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691612456044.
Dittmar, H., & Drury, J. (2000). Self-image – is it in the bag? A qualitative comparison between ‘ordinary’ and ‘excessive’ consumers. Journal of Economic Psychology, 21(2), 109–142. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0167-4870(00)00002-0.
Lee, C. M., Neighbors, C., & Woods, B. A. (2007). Marijuana motives: Young adults’ reasons for using marijuana. Addictive Behaviors, 32(7), 1384–1394. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2006.09.010.
Li, J., Kaltiainen, J., & Hakanen, J. J. (2024). Job boredom as an antecedent of four states of mental health: Life satisfaction, positive functioning, anxiety, and depression symptoms among young employees—A latent change score approach. BMC Public Health, 24(1), 907. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-024-18430-z.
Mercer, K. B., & Eastwood, J. D. (2010). Is boredom associated with problem gambling behaviour? It depends on what you mean by ‘boredom’. International Gambling Studies, 10(1), 91–104. https://doi.org/10.1080/14459791003754414.
Ndetei, D. M., Nyamai, P., & Mutiso, V. (2023). Boredom—Understanding the emotion and its impact on our lives: An African perspective. Frontiers in Sociology, 8, 1213190. https://doi.org/10.3389/fsoc.2023.1213190.
O’Hanlon, J. F. (1981). Boredom: Practical consequences and a theory. Acta Psychologica, 49, 53–82. https://doi.org/10.1016/0001-6918(81)90033-0.
Olié, E., Dubois, J., Benramdane, M., Guillaume, S., & Courtet, P. (2022). Poor mental health is associated with loneliness and boredom during COVID-19-related restriction periods in patients with pre-existing depression. Journal of Affective Disorders, 319, 446–461. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2022.09.040.
Steele, R., Henderson, P., Lennon, F., & Swinden, D. (2013). Boredom among psychiatric in-patients: Does it matter? Advances in Psychiatric Treatment, 19(4), 259–267. https://doi.org/10.1192/apt.bp.112.010363.
Tam, K. Y. Y., Chan, C. S., van Tilburg, W. A. P., Lavi, I., & Lau, J. Y. F. (2023). Boredom belief moderates the mental health impact of boredom among young people: Correlational and multi-wave longitudinal evidence gathered during the COVID-19 pandemic. Journal of Personality, 91(3), 638–652. https://doi.org/10.1111/jopy.12764
Todman, M. (2003). Boredom and psychotic disorders: Cognitive and motivational issues. Psychiatry, 66(2), 146–167. https://doi.org/10.1521/psyc.66.2.146.20623.
van Hooft, E. A. J., & van Hooff, M. L. M. (2018). The state of boredom: Frustrating or depressing? Motivation and Emotion, 42(6), 931–946. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-018-9710-6.
van Tilburg, W. A., Igou, E. R., & Sedikides, C. (2013). In search of meaningfulness: Nostalgia as an antidote to boredom. Emotion, 13(3), 450–461. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030442.
Weiss, E. R., Todman, M., Maple, E., & Bunn, R. R. (2022). Boredom in a time of uncertainty: State and trait boredom associations with psychological health during COVID-19. Behavioral Sciences, 12(8), 298. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs12080298